Image source
There’s no shortage of odd, confusing, infuriating, ill-founded and contentious claims in education, and in the wider social chattersphere that sits around it, but surely there’s one that we can put to bed for good and for all: the idea that it is the job of parents to teach their children how to read – and by extension, that parents are to blame when children struggle in this endeavour.
There are many reasons why this cosy, middle-class-meme-cum-system-level-excuse-for-schools needs to be exploded and I’ll address as many as I can here.
First of all though, I think we can agree that reading is a fundamentally important life-skill and one that is best fostered in the early years of school. Reading is the means by which students engage with the academic curriculum and have the opportunity to succeed at school. The downstream benefits of successful early reading achievement yield the kinds of investment returns we can only dream of for our financial portfolios. When students become proficient readers in a timely and smooth way, they are more likely to succeed academically. When students achieve academically, they are more likely to display prosocial, on-task behaviour in classrooms. When students display prosocial, on-task classroom behaviours, they are more likely to feel a sense of attachment to school, to stay at school, and to exit secondary school with open doors to postsecondary training, education and/or employment. When schools have early reading instruction nailed, they are also going a long way to addressing student wellbeing. It’s hard to be self-regulated, calm, and ready-to-learn if you are locked out of the writing system by which learning is transacted in classrooms (see references at the end of this post).
Whole Language (WL) and its descendent ideology, Balanced Literacy (BL) are based on ideas about reading and reading instruction that have not withstood rigorous scrutiny over the last three decades. This scrutiny includes cognitive psychology research as well as system-level data about reading performance. Key among the premises of WL/BL instruction is the notion that reading, like oral language, is a “natural” thing to do. The problem here is that this is patently incorrect. If reading was “natural”, we would not have hundreds of thousands of illiterate or semi-literate adults in the world (who, incidentally, in most cases, have been to school).
The work of US evolutionary psychologist David Geary has been pivotal in illuminating the important distinction between human knowledge and skills that are biologically primary and those that are biologically secondary. Biologically primary skills include acquiring oral language, learning to walk, learning to read facial expressions, and displaying reciprocity in social interactions. Biologically secondary skills, on the other hand, are skills that we humans have the capacity to learn but need to be taught. Here we are looking at reading, writing and spelling, mastering mathematical concepts such as algebra and trigonometry, learning musical instruments, and becoming proficient in activities such as chess, golf, and driving a car. Many biologically secondary skills are developed through classroom instruction at school (though the pedagogical approach adopted will unfortunately result in variable outcomes).
WL/BL proponents did not consider the fact that humans have had oral language for a couple of hundred thousand years, thanks to advantageous evolutionary changes in our brains, but writing systems for only approximately five thousand years. They did not reflect on the fact that writing is a social, albeit clever contrivance, developed by humans to share information horizontally with each other, and vertically, with future generations. To do this, humans had to create a code to represent oral language, and speakers of different languages did this over thousands of years by developing an orthography for their particular language. English, for historical reasons, has one of the more complex alphabetic writing codes in the world. Unfortunately, the lack of sophistication inherent in WL/BL thinking meant that this was taken to mean that English writing is too “cumbersome and unreliable” to teach explicitly. But conveniently, that didn't matter, because they decided to call reading, writing, and spelling “natural skills” that children could pick up via rich exposure and skills in “prediction” in their environments. Problem solved. Or not.
This smoke-and-mirrors sleight of epistemological hand resulted in a staggering dumbing down of the reading and writing process by faculties of education in English-speaking worlds because they now (apparently) did not need to devote preservice hours to explaining the intricacies of the English writing system to teachers, let alone to equipping them with instructional practices that would maximise mastery by the overwhelming majority of students, regardless of their preschool oral language experiences. Conveniently, this meant that academics did not have to have background-knowledge in linguistics or cognitive psychology. “Reading” (a verb) was replaced by the abstract noun “literacy” and was re-imagined as a vague social and cultural process that students experience through immersion in rich, so-called “authentic” texts. Just as children develop oral language through interactions with others, a misguided folklore took hold in education that this is how they can and should become readers.
This folklore did a number of clever things for academics in schools of education:
· It meant that education academics could indulge their own love of children’s literature and build entire subjects (units or courses, depending on terminology) around this and pretend that this was legitimate preparation for classroom practice with children who are in many cases complete novices with written text.
· It justified education academics’ withholding of critical knowledge from preservice teachers about how the English writing system works. I have described this knowledge being like teachers’ family china. It was removed, piece-by-precious-piece, and replaced with what looked like more modern, but plastic wares that do not stand the test of time.
· It conflated books that children read as novices with books that adults read to children. Where books were considered to have an instructional role, these were predictable and promoted reliance on cues outside the text such as pictures, because there was (apparently) really no difference between oral language and reading. This means that an illusion of early reading could be created cleverly and quickly in the first year of school, by having children draw on their oral language skills and basic vocabulary knowledge to recite predictable texts. This is very different from the usually slow and effortful grinding of cognitive and linguistic gears that occurs when children are mastering an unfamiliar skill-set through explicit teaching of how the code works, e.g., through systematic synthetic phonics instruction. It is initially slow, faltering, and hesitant, in the same way that a novice learning a musical instrument is slow, faltering, and hesitant, or anyone mastering any complex, biologically secondary skill through practice and repetition is slow, faltering, and hesitant.
· Perhaps most perniciously, it meant that parents could be positioned as the primary agents of reading instruction, so that when children did not successfully become readers after three years of formal schooling, responsibility could be laid squarely at the feet of parents and the impugned quality of the home language and literacy environment. Teachers, schools, education systems, policy makers and education academics could all look the other way. Shame on those parents who obviously did not read to their children from the moment of birth. What did they expect? That schools would somehow turn around their neglectful parenting practices? This gaslighting of parents has unfortunately been supported by some high-profile children’s authors.
So, what IS the role of parents in their children’s early reading success?
What all policy-makers, educators, and clinicians would ideally like to see, is children being raised in language and text-rich home environments, supported by high-quality preschool and childcare experiences. We have known for a long time, however, that there is a social gradient on which families sit with respect to the extent to which they can support children’s early oral language development. This is not a free pass for schools to shrug their shoulders, however. On the contrary, it is a reminder that oral language itself requires a focus in early years classrooms.
The role of parents in reading instruction is, all other factors being equal and within their capacities:
· To engage in language rich interactions, elaborating on children’s utterances, teaching new vocabulary, and the meanings of common idioms.
· To model reading as a valued, worthwhile activity in their own lives.
· To read to their children (ideally even after they can read for themselves).
· To provide access to quality children’s books (in the home and via library visits).
· To listen to their children reading instructional texts they bring home from school, providing feedback and encouragement. Parents reading to their children should not be mistaken for the process by which children learn to read, any more than parents playing classical music to their children should be conflated with teaching them how to play a musical instrument. Reading to children should be enjoyable for parents and children and can promote oral language skills and background knowledge. These contribute to the language reservoir that children will draw on at school when the serious business of learning how speech and print map to each other should be one of several early instructional focus areas.
Where parents can provide some or all of the above, this is a bonus and should be encouraged, but should not be relied upon as a principal means by which reading success will be chalked up by schools.
Sadly, given the continued stronghold of BL instruction in most English-speaking countries, it is also advisable for parents to critically interrogate reading instruction approaches being employed in their children’s classrooms.
Parents sometimes unwittingly conceal the real ways that their children achieve success and in so-doing enable schools and systems to cruise, falsely reassured that their BL diet is producing healthy outcomes. In many cases in fact, these are coming from supplements provided by parents, directly at the kitchen table, or indirectly via investment in tutors. If credit is taken by schools for reading success, then responsibility must also be taken for casualties. This requires an honest look at what is going on in the school community and the extent to which parents are ironically, propping up reading success rather than being the scoundrels who neglect this aspect of their child’s development.
Let’s not overlook too, the aspect of teacher well-being
and professionalism
that resides in the knowledge that they are competent and accountable professionals
with respect to this critical aspect of their everyday work. In a time of severe workforce shortages, teacher self-efficacy regarding children's reading success is surely a win we can all celebrate.
When a child is struggling with early reading, questions do need to be asked and answered.
The wrong question is the one directed at parents and is some version of “Do/did you read to her at home?”
The right question for teachers and schools to ask is “Why is this child not progressing and how can we adjust our instruction to address this”?
Effective reading instruction by teachers in the first three
years of school is like planting a tree that their students will sit under as
adults. Teachers themselves may not see the enormity of the leafy canopy their
efforts have fostered, but we can count the costs of early reading instruction
casualties by tallying up justice, mental health, substance abuse, public
housing, and other social services for adults with low literacy skills.
Further reading (all open access)
Snow, P. (2020). Balanced literacy or systematic reading instruction? Perspectives on Language and Literacy, Winter, 35-39.
Snow, P. (2020). SOLAR: The Science of Language and Reading. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 37(3), 222–233.
Dear Parents: Welcome to the Confusing World of Reading Instruction
Pamela Snow © 2023
THANKYou Professor Pam! 🙏🏻
ReplyDeleteThis is a glorious read. Being a child that failed to learn to read , then having a child that also failed, makes this a very poignant read. This is the premise as to why I continue to advocate for change . You are so generous with your knowledge
Sandra people like your good self know this space more intimately than anyone. Thank you for all that you do in spite of the awful associations your experiences have for you.
DeleteWhat a timely read. The parent blaming and the whole "waiting for their light bulb moment' method of reading facilitation are my top 2 annoyances at the moment. I happily reject both when schools try to slip them in when I'm providing assessment feedback sessions, but I fear I will end up in hot water for being a little too enthusiastic in my calling them out one day.
ReplyDeleteThanks for your comment Scott. Unfortunately, I think crossing their fingers and hoping for the best is a common response from teachers whose initial teacher education has let them down. When teachers learn to be more explicit and intentional in their reading instruction, this kind of magical thinking is one of the first things they let go of.
DeleteApologies Scott - that was me replying to your post. I accidentally did so anonymously :-)
Delete"We have known for a long time, however, that there is a social gradient on which families sit with respect to the extent to which they can support children’s early oral language development. This is not a free pass for schools to shrug their shoulders, however. On the contrary, it is a reminder that oral language itself requires a focus in early years classrooms."
ReplyDeleteI would add that it is a reminder that because we are making up for lost time, we must carefully choose our reading activities to maximize efficiency and effectiveness. I became aware of this urgency after discovering the research by Anne Fernald with infants 18-24 months related to child-directed speech, which impacts vocabulary knowledge and processing speed and future reading proficiency. We need to understand who our students are and act with both urgency and intentionality to provide the instruction they need.
What a comprehensive argument (and simultaneous take-down) for what to teach and why! Thank you so much.
I couldn't endorse your comment about urgency more Harriet. The instructional environment is the (only) one that teachers can influence, but some are unfortunately taught to have a nihilistic view of just how much impact they can have. This then becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
DeleteThis is confusing.
ReplyDeleteIn the end, either the simple view of reading is right or it isn't. If it is, then the kind of vocabulary and syntax learning that home reading surely facilitates must be important for learning reading, and the failure of some families to do this a significant part of the issue.
In the more fervid writings in support of phonics, there is a constant equivocation between 'reading' and 'decoding' , so that the latter is surreptitiously taken to be 'how one learns to read' in spite of the fact that, if the sor (the theory they themselves advocate) is right, it by definition isn't.
Indeed, in the UK a dramatic improvement in phonics know-how over the last ten years has led to negligible long-term improvement in reading. To explain this, hard-phonics advocates have fallen back on rather confused analogies with steering wheels and marathons, but I think they are ultimately going to have to come to terms with the fact that other factors such as, say, levels of home reading, are turning out to be more important than the degree and type of phonics drilling in the first 24 months (though, to be clear, as a soft-phonics person, I still reckon phonics drilling for the first twelve months is probably the best approach we yet know of)
Statement from the UK government: “Since the introduction of the phonics screening check in 2012, the percentage of Year 1 pupils meeting the expected standard in reading has risen from 58% to 82%, with 92% of children achieving this standard by Year 2.”
Delete"In the end, either the simple view of reading is right or it isn't. If it is, then the kind of vocabulary and syntax learning that home reading surely facilitates must be important for learning reading, and the failure of some families to do this a significant part of the issue."
DeleteMaybe, sure. I read constantly to my kid. But my kid didn't really get going with reading, steadily, until after intensive Barton tutoring. I learned too late that Units of Study is just garbage.
I now have my kids in a private school that uses an SoR aligned curriculum.
Anonymous, I’m really not sure why calling out parent blame is confusing in relation to the integrity of the Simple View of Reading (SVR). The SVR is understandably silent about where or how children develop skills and knowledge on both sides of its formula. It merely demonstrates that they must do so. So, this means that gains that are made at home, at school, at the local library, at grandparents’ homes and so on, will all contribute to reading proficiency. My point in this post is that it is fallacious and inequitable (to say the least) to sheet home responsibility to parents for teaching reading, when it is clearly in the remit of schools. Parents vary considerably with respect to the extent to which they can support their children’s early and ongoing literacy progress. Some parents are themselves illiterate. Do we just accept that this will be “inherited” by the next generation? Can schools only be expected to be successful with students who were going to get there anyway? Is it OK for schools to take credit for gains achieved through tutoring sourced and paid for by parents?
DeleteYes, there have been some changes in reading instruction in England since the adoption of the Rose Report recommendations, most notably the mandating of systematic synthetic phonics instruction, and assessment of children’s decoding skills via the Year 1 Phonics Screening Check. This does not mean, however, that there has been a wholesale move away from Balanced Literacy or that other aspects of the reading process (such as vocabulary, fluency, comprehension etc) are being addressed any better than they were prior to the commencement of the PSC a bit over a decade ago (the reality is that we don’t know much about what actually goes on in classrooms). Improving decoding is but one part of a larger, more complex picture and we can’t expect this one skill to bear the weight of reading comprehension – which of course leads us back to the SVR. All of that said, England is trending up in PIRLS and Australia is trending down. The release of the next round of data in May will be interesting.
Hi Doctor snow.
ReplyDeleteDon't you think there's even a small risk that your rhetoric could be construed as teacher blaming? I think that would be unfair, in fact, but it's also unfair to say that mem fox was parent blaming.
The incredible achievements of English primary teachers I think deserve more recognition - massive improvements in the phonics screening test results took a huge amount of work, and to say now that its failure to produce tangible longterm results is because they're still not doing enough seems a little like deflection.
I guess at the heart of our disagreement is the certainty that you feel that 'reading' is the 'school's remit'. Given the extent to which it depends on language development, and the extent to which language is a deeply social phenomenon, I think that rather utopian. I suspect that part of the explanation for the viciousness of the reading wars is that they are conducted by two groups suffering from utopian ambitions for the school system. .
Joe pugh
Dear Joe
DeleteSorry about my slow reply. I work with literally thousands of teachers, across primary, secondary and specialist settings. I have enormous respect for the complexity and importance of the work they do, and regularly tell them this. Expressing frustration with their pre-service preparation is not teacher-bashing and in fact it is invariably welcomed by teachers, as they have often had their own epiphany about this, and all I am doing is putting some words around their experience. Mem Fox has actively campaigned over the years against children being explicitly taught how their writing system works.
I actually think it's utopian to think that all parents can share the heavy lifting on children's reading success. I really wish they could, but the socioeconomic imbalance in this means that schools have to be able to be relied upon to step up. I agree that everyone in this debate wants better outcomes for students.
Thanks for engaging,
Pam
Thanks for replying Dr Snow.
ReplyDeleteI'd agree entirely that it would be utopian to simply 'expect' parents do the heavy lifting - as you say, socio-economic factors render that frankly punitive (and, if Mem Fox suggested otherwise, I would join you in condemning her). Rather, I'm a bit of an old-fashioned materialist: address inequalities in housing, work conditions and pay, and allow social and cultural effects to flow from that (though you'd probably think this utopian!).
I have to admit I paused for thought when the PIRLS results came out - England did do well, and perhaps the phonics boost did indeed contribute to this (so too, I reckon, did the English kids doing their tests a whole year later than most participants).
Still, I do think that the 'hard phonics' crowd (including the UK government) have once again exaggerated the results. Excluding England, the mean COVID effect was -7 scale points from the 2016 score, suggesting phonics has boosted England's score by 6 scale points. Without this, England would have ended up 4th anyway.
This reasoning is pretty crude and close to unscientific, but so is claiming that the results definitively demonstrate the transformative power of SSP (something Greg Ashman, to his credit, acknowledges in his blog).
Again, to be clear, my position is that drilling total novices with GPCs is the certianly best way yet to introduce people to reading English. It's just that NOT doing this is a long way from being the main reason Jonny can't read. This view is also given some support by the PIRLS report, whose headline finding is that 'Home support has a substantial impact on reading achievement' -
https://www.iea.nl/news-events/news/pirls-2021-international-report-and-results-now-online).
Again, thanks too for engaging without that yah-boo stuff - I really appreciate your respectful tone
Joe
Thanks for replying Dr Snow.
ReplyDeleteI'd agree entirely that it would be utopian to simply 'expect' parents do the heavy lifting - as you say, socio-economic factors render that frankly punitive (and, if Mem Fox suggested otherwise, I would join you in condemning her). Rather, I'm a bit of an old-fashioned materialist: address inequalities in housing, work conditions and pay, and allow social and cultural effects to flow from that (though you'd probably think this utopian!).
I have to admit I paused for thought when the PIRLS results came out - England did do well, and perhaps the phonics boost did indeed contribute to this (so too, I reckon, did the English kids doing their tests a whole year later than most participants).
Still, I do think that the 'hard phonics' crowd (including the UK government) have once again exaggerated the results. Excluding England, the mean COVID effect was -7 scale points from the 2016 score, suggesting phonics has boosted England's score by 6 scale points. Without this, England would have ended up 4th anyway.
This reasoning is pretty crude and close to unscientific, but so is claiming that the results definitively demonstrate the transformative power of SSP (something Greg Ashman, to his credit, acknowledges in his blog).
Again, to be clear, my position is that drilling total novices with GPCs is the certianly best way yet to introduce people to reading English. It's just that NOT doing this is a long way from being the main reason Jonny can't read. This view is also given some support by the PIRLS report, whose headline finding is that 'Home support has a substantial impact on reading achievement' -
https://www.iea.nl/news-events/news/pirls-2021-international-report-and-results-now-online).
Again, thanks too for engaging without that yah-boo stuff - I really appreciate your respectful tone
Joe
When you say "my position is that drilling total novices with GPCs is the certianly best way yet to introduce people to reading English", I'm sure you didn't mean it to come across as mechanistic as it does, especially since one of the criticisms from some parts is that phonics instruction is just 'drill and kill'. Clearly, there's a lot that we're up against, and integrating child-friendly phonics instruction into a much larger, more comprehensive literacy program is a huge part of helping Johnny read.
Delete