A couple of weeks ago, I posted a low-key self-assessment rubric, mainly as a conversation-starter for teachers and schools about current practices,
knowledge, and beliefs pertaining to the teaching of phonics to beginning
readers. Quite a few people contacted me with their views, and asked me to post
"answers" to the questions I posed.
So here goes:
1. Does your reading
instruction begin with the introduction of a small number of letter-sound
correspondences, and explicitly (and gradually) teach children how to blend,
segment, insert, and delete sounds in order to produce different words?
|
This approach is at the heart
of systematic synthetic phonics
(SSP). As noted in my previous blogpost, there are different approaches to
phonics instruction, but they are not equally effective for beginning
readers, and SSP is most likely to see more children off to a good start,
especially those children whose progress is likely to be compromised by known
or unknown vulnerabilities. Interested readers are referred to this excellent overview by A/Prof
Deslea Konza, from ECU University.
I am aware that some teachers
say they find beginning reading instruction in this way “boring”. I have a
pretty simple response to such teachers: in the nicest possible way, this is
not about your needs, it’s about the needs of young children. I have seen
great examples of early years teachers making SSP engaging and rewarding, so
this really doesn’t wash. Perhaps teachers just need to feel more comfortable
and confident with the approach.
|
2. Do you use decodable texts
as the starting point for children starting to read, or predictable texts?
|
It is one thing for children
to be introduced to the process of reading via SSP, but it is another thing
altogether for that initial teaching to be associated with the use of initial
decodable texts. Decodable texts are pretty much what it says on the packet: books
that contain words that are easily decodable by beginning readers. Such books
provide opportunities to consolidate emergent knowledge of sound-letter
links, so that these become more automatic for the beginning reader.
There are many
commercially-available sets of decodable readers, such as Fitzroy Readers, Little Learners Love
Literacy, Dandelion
Readers, and Pocket
Rockets.
If your school has made the
move to SSP, well done – that’s a significant step towards ensuring that more
children emerge from the first three years of school as confident and
competent readers. If, however, you are juxtaposing SSP with predictable
texts, it is likely that your students are receiving mixed messages about the
reading process, and may, as a consequence, develop some unhelpful early
habits, such as “reading” from picture cues.
I have blogged
previously about the uncritical acceptance of the importance of “authentic
texts” for beginning readers. What does “authentic” even mean, and who
decides which texts will be anointed with this descriptor? These are big
questions that can sit on the table for now.
|
3. Do you employ a so-called
“Three Cueing” process in which beginning readers are encouraged to “guess”
unknown words they encounter while reading, with attention to the first
letter and its corresponding sound used as a last resort?
|
Three-cueing is a Whole
Language zombie that can be found in many Australian classrooms. How many? Who
knows, but it comes up often enough in my discussions with classroom teachers
and parents, for me to be of the view that it is alive and well. I am happy
to be presented with evidence to the contrary.
I encourage you to read Alison
Clarke’s terrific summary as to why it is not aligned with SSP teaching.
In fact, as pointed out by Professor Mark Seidenberg, in his 2016
publication, Language at the Speed
of Sight, the Three Cueing strategy simply teaches the habits of poor
readers, and why would we want to do that?
|
4. Do you deal with
sound-letter correspondences only “in context” rather than teaching these in
isolation as a starting point?
|
As you would realise, SSP has
a focus on early teaching of specific skills, in isolation, to reduce the
cognitive and linguistic load imposed on young children early on in the
reading process.
Any reference to teaching
reading skills in isolation typically elicits protests from the Whole
Language die-hards who fervently maintain that meaning making is at the
heart of reading, or words to that effect. Meaning making is certainly at
the heart of reading for skilled readers, but beginning readers need to
familiarise themselves with the apparatus first.
|
5. Do you introduce lists of
so-called “tricky” words (sight-words) to be learnt as wholes, early on in
the reading instruction process?
|
The ultimate aim of reading
is for most, if not all words, to become “sight words” – words that are
instantly, and in fact, unavoidably recognised and understood as wholes.
No-one wants to engage in the laborious and unrewarding task of sounding out
every word in a sentence. That would result in poor fluency, low
comprehension and a generally unrewarding experience.
However, that does not mean
that the “fast track” to filling children’s brains with sight words is to
present them with lists of words (in many cases with at least one
irregularity in phoneme-grapheme correspondence from the perspective of the
beginning reader), and requiring them to memorise them.
By all means, introduce sight
words, but do so systematically, and via discussion with children about their
composition. Most sight words contain some regular features, plus one or more
irregular features. These can be discussed with and explained to beginning
readers, as part of the word-study process and the wonder of learning about
the origins of the English language.
|
6. Do you base your reading
instruction around some version of “The
Big Five” (phonics, phonemic awareness, vocabulary, comprehension, and
fluency?
|
It must always be stressed
that effective phonics instruction, and the knowledge of the alphabetic principle that it confers, is necessary but not sufficient for beginning
readers. Beginning readers also need to be developing their phonemic awareness
skills, vocabulary, comprehension, and fluency. Some authors, such as A/Prof
Deslea Konza, argue that we should upgrade the “Big Five” to the “Big Six”,
to specifically include oracy- see
full-text of her excellent paper here. As a speech pathologist, I can’t
argue with this position. Broader language skills (e.g. with respect to
narrative language) contribute to, and are strengthened by, effective reading
skills. Language and literacy have a symbiotic relationship, as I have described
in detail in this open access 2015 paper.
|
7. Do you focus instruction
around a “Letter of the Week”?
|
Many students arrive at
school “knowing their letters”, e.g. as evidenced by the ability to recite
the alphabet and/or to sing the Alphabet Song (the one that is sung to the
tune of Twinkle, Twinkle, Little Star, and annoyingly for us folk outside
North America, has the final letter pronounced “Zee”). Knowledge of these 26
letters is a vital starting point, but as they need to work together to
represent the 44 sounds that are used in English, teaching via a letter of
the week is slow and cumbersome, and sometimes confusing for beginning
readers. You can read more
about this here.
On the subject of letters, I was reminded too, by one of my Tweeps, that letters do not "make" or "say" sounds - they represent sounds, and often work in combination to do so, e.g., in digraphs such as th, ch, and sh. |
8. Do teachers in your school
discourage parents from helping their children “sound out” words they do not
know when they are reading at home?
|
It should be apparent from
all I have written above that this is not a recommended approach. Parents are
natural teachers of their children and are accustomed to breaking complex
tasks down into manageable chunks. That is exactly the approach they should
be encouraged to take with respect to early reading.
Sending decodable books home
with parents encourages this natural approach and has a hidden benefit for
parents whose literacy levels are low* – it makes the process of supporting
their young child more enjoyable and achievable.
*This is also helpful to parents from non English-speaking backgrounds, who may, themselves, be learning to read in English. |
9. Do teachers in your school
tell students that English is a “random” language (or words to that effect)?
|
This is yet another piece of
Whole Language excess baggage and is incorrect. As
I have explained in an earlier blogpost:
About 50% of English words do have a
transparent orthography, meaning that they can be read by someone who
understands letter—sound correspondences. A further 36% have only one sound
that deviates (typically a vowel), 10% can be spelt correctly if morphology
and etymology are understood, and a mere 4% cannot be decoded from knowledge
of these principles.
Literacy experts should not
be promulgating this myth.
The "irregular" aspects of English spelling create more, not less need for explicit and systematic teaching. |
I would be very pleased to add to / amend these responses on the basis of reader comments and feedback. The purpose of this blog is, after all, to share information and ideas.
******************
******************
You may also be interested in The Story of an Ugly Duckling: aka Phonics Check Furphies
Yes yes yes!! Thanks so much for putting so much crucial information into such a concise and easy to read format. I would love to just print this out and deliver it to every school in our area with a post it note saying: "Let this be your guide to making changes!!". With regards to point 5- I would love it if you or some of your very talented blog readers could provide some guidance or links to articles/blog posts that outline how to introduce sight words in a systematic manner, and with regards to whichever sight words are recommended to introduce first, some tips on how to explain the composition of these early sight words to our students who are very much only at the 'beginning reader' stage.
ReplyDeleteThanks for your encouraging response - I hope this post is of some use to you in your conversations with colleagues.
ReplyDeleteOn the subject of sight words, have a look at this by Alison Clarke, on her Spelfabet site:
https://www.spelfabet.com.au/2012/07/teaching-sight-words/
This was written by Professor Anne Castles (Macquarie University) and aroused quite some discussion at the time:
http://readoxford.org/guest-blog-are-sight-words-unjustly-slighted
Alison Clarke then wrote this piece in response to the debate around Anne Castle's piece:
https://www.spelfabet.com.au/2016/07/dissing-choosing-and-teaching-sight-words/
Happy reading!
cheers
Pam
Thank you for your wonderful blog posts, Pamela. I am a school-based SLP in Canada trying my best to advocate for SSP reading instruction. Your posts inspire me to keep at it! I will be printing off this post to share with some of my resource teacher colleagues.
ReplyDeleteI am so pleased to hear my posts are of use to you in your work, half a world away in Canada AM. Please keep in touch :-)
ReplyDeletecheers
Pam
As a teacher who was trained in "Balanced Literacy" at University, the journey to self educate in SSP has been challenging at times. My school leadership has been wonderfully supportive in providing PD opportunities and the resources that allow me to introduce my students to the process of reading in a structured and explicit way, and yet I still feel I have so much to discover. I really appreciate the accessability of your blog posts and how informative they are, particularly when I try to share my knowledge with my colleagues. As I (try to) keep up with the writings of Alison Clarke, Prof. Anne Castles, Dr Jennifer Buckingham at FivefromFive and Assoc. Prof. Deslea Konza, I can't help but wonder if there are any other Academics that are from Education faculties who advocate for a SSP approach to reading instruction? Are any teachers in Australia graduating with this knowledge?
ReplyDeleteI have my parent hat on in this response. I am a secondary teacher. However everyone of the questions on the left is what my child is being taught at his school. It fact i have ditched is levelled readers and am using decodable books at home. In fact when they were teaching sight words (which I then begin to discuss in terms of decoding- and the exceptions) I was told he couldn't read the sight words (because he was using the decoding strategies I had taught him). Luckily my son comes from a high literacy home and he is doing well in his reading. But many problems are faced by children who come from low literacy backgrounds.
ReplyDeleteThanks for your insightful comment Liz and my apologies for the tardy reply - I don't always see these notifications in a timely manner. Good on you for trusting your own judgement - your son's reading skills will be the stronger for it in this case.
DeleteAll the best
Pam
Hi Pamela
ReplyDeleteThis is a great checklist. I would like to add, ' Does your school discourage children from pointing to the words with a finger while they read?' Pointing to words helps children pay attention to the words and to blend phonemes left to right in proper sequence. So many of my students are ordered not to point to the words as it is 'babyish'.
Thanks for your comment Antonia and apologies that I've been slow in acknowledging it. You raise an interesting issue, though it's not one that comes up a lot in discussions, as far as I can see. However I agree with you that pointing with the finger is a probably just a sensible form of self-guidance/scaffolding, that most children will spontaneously stop doing once they become more fluent as readers. I certainly wouldn't be discouraging it, and as for the "babyish" label - well I'd remind people who say that that babies don't actually read, so it's a nonsense in that way, though 5-6 year olds are "baby readers" and so may need extra supports to get going.
ReplyDeleteAll the best
Pam