Friday 1 September 2023

Balanced literacy and New Zealand’s opportunity to re-write reading instruction history.

 

This week, I was privileged to speak alongside Emily Hanford (American Public Media; Sold a Story) and Associate Professor Lorraine Hammond, AM (Edith Cowan University)  at the inaugural Cultivating the Literacy Landscape symposia in New Zealand (NZ); one in Christchurch, the other in Auckland. These events were auspiced by the NZ NGO, Learning Matters, under the outstanding leadership of Founder and Managing Director, Carla McNeil. Carla is a former teacher and school principal, as well as being the parent of a young adult who experienced significant learning challenges. She is well-qualified therefore, to comment on the literacy landscape in her homeland.

Readers of this blog and subscribers to the Sold a Story podcast will be well aware that New Zealand has played a key role since the 1970s in ensuring the stronghold of whole language (more latterly, balanced literacy) instruction and support, both in initial teacher education and in school policy and practice, through the influential work, nationally and internationally, of the late Dame Marie Clay.

In an unfortunate but not uncommon case of national cognitive dissonance, NZ’s literacy rates in recent decades do not provide a basis for celebrating Clay’s legacy. To be clear, Australia’s performance with respect to literacy achievement is also nothing to crow about (see also here), with students from disadvantaged backgrounds carrying an unfair burden associated with high-variance teaching. This high-variance, in turn, derives from a stubborn reluctance by higher education providers to end their romantic devotion to balanced literacy and commit to changing the knowledge and skills that undergraduate teaching students receive in exchange for a higher education debt. I have blogged previously about the resistance of higher education providers to engaging with evidence, and the glacial rate of change in that sector as a consequence.

After more than two decades, there’s an inconvenient truth, however, for education faculties and policy makers to face:

Balanced literacy has been an ill-conceived, poorly designed, inadequately monitored, and hence unethical, social experiment.

We would all baulk at revelations of hospitals going rogue in their treatment of child patients, and each taking their own approach to diagnostic assessments and interventions. In rare cases where substandard care does occur, there is community outrage and public naming and shaming of hospitals, if not individual staff. This level of accountability does not apply in education. If education academics want to truly endow education with the status of an esteemed profession, then preparing graduates to be evidence-based and accountable practitioners would be an excellent place to start.

Sadly, in many western, industrialised nations, low respect for the rights and needs of children has enabled vital education protections and checks to be eroded, in favour of the rights of adults to indulge their own ideas and preferences with respect to classroom practice.

Balanced literacy has been the perfect Petrie dish for cultivating eclecticism in reading instruction. It asks next to nothing of education academics in terms of understanding decades of cognitive psychology research on the nature of the reading process and sharing this with the next generation of classroom teachers. Education academics in turn, have exploited this freedom by busying themselves with their own preferred patches of garden, in authentic children’s literature, digital literacy, critical literacy, multi-literacies, and so on.  Teaching reading, however, must be about the time-sensitive needs of children, not the aesthetic preferences and ideologies of adults.

In my keynote presentation at the NZ symposia, I suggested that it’s time for us to have a conversation (some “hard words” perhaps) with balanced literacy and in so doing, engage in some awkward fact-checking.

Here’s a potted summary of what I argued that education academics and policy makers need to come to grips with, as a matter of urgency:

Awkward reality

Implications, fall-out, and discussion points

Putting adjectives in front of the word “literacy” is not an acceptable substitute for teaching children how to read.

Reading is a verb, and literacy is a noun. That means it is something that children need to be able to do. It is not a vague and abstract concept that can have countless adjectives casually placed in front of it (digital, critical, legal, health, maths, etc) to appease the interests of adults. If students can read, write, and spell, there is every chance that they can develop and display multiple forms of literacy. It does not work the other way.

Balanced literacy needs to wipe the Vaseline off its lens and address the fact that children need to be able to read before they can become literate.

Reading is a biologically secondary (“unnatural”) thing for humans to do.

Humans have had oral language skills for approximately 200K years but writing systems for only 3-5K years. Reading and writing are social contrivances that happen to carry much currency in contemporary industrialised nations.

As numerous cognitive psychologists have noted (e.g., Harvard’s Steven Pinker), we have a language brain, not a reading brain. We can acquire a reading brain, but most of us need exposure to high quality instruction in the early years of school in order to do so. Many other biologically secondary skills also need to be developed in the context of school.

Balanced literacy does not have a plan for explicitly and systematically teaching the life-changing skills of reading and writing to all children, regardless of their starting point. The fact that some children get across the bridge from oral language to reading and writing proficiency via balanced literacy does not justify its use as a population-level reading instruction approach.

English has one of the most complex alphabetic writing systems in the world

English has a history of rich borrowings of vocabulary, spellings, and sentence structure from other languages. This reflects centuries of invasions, trade patterns, religious influences, inter-marrying of royal houses, and even the Black Death.

Spelling is not “the problem” in English; the extent to which pronunciation is free to shift around is the culprit. Spelling changes slowly, where pronunciation can differ between two members of the same household. Spellings in turn, reflect their ancestral roots, or etymologies, but teachers rarely learn about this in their pre-service education.

Balanced literacy has dealt with the rich and complex history of English by ignoring it. It’s been a case of “if you don’t understand it, don’t vote for it”.

Teachers need to be knowledgeable about how their writing system works.

This point is related to the one above. When I go into a school, I want to find that the staff who are the most knowledgeable and confident about what reading is, how to teach it, how to monitor progress, how to intervene for struggling students are….you guessed it….. teachers. Sadly, this is often not the case.

There is a raft of international evidence indicating low levels of teacher knowledge of critical language constructs that underpin reading instruction  (see references on this page).

Explicit knowledge about how their writing system works is the precious family china that has been systematically eroded from teachers’ possession in recent decades. Balanced literacy, by definition, is not in a position to reinstate this china; when teachers do re-claim it, however, they are encouraged to use their best china every day, for all students, not just those who are struggling. 

Reading is a language-based task, but strong oral language skills are not enough.

This relates to the fact that oral language is a biologically primary skill-set and reading, writing, and spelling are biologically secondary, and so need to be taught (and taught well).

We want all children to arrive at school with well-developed oral language skills, expressively and receptively, across vocabulary, sentence structure and morphology, discourse, inferential language, and so forth.

Balanced literacy wants to leverage this to create an illusion of early reading fluency through children’s recitation of predictable texts, instead of giving children early code mastery so they can foster their ongoing oral language skills through the endless well of opportunities afforded by timely reading proficiency.

We should be successfully teaching 95% of children to read, not 60-70%.

This is a point made by a number of reading scientists, including Dr Kerry Hempenstall in this 2013 academic paper. When reading is taught effectively and efficiently at Tier 1, achievement levels of the whole class go up, and fewer students need to draw on the precious few intervention resources that can be mobilised to support them if they fall behind. This is what Response to Intervention is all about. RTI, in turn, sits within the broader Multi Tiered Systems of Support framework, as synthesised by Dr Kate de Bruin and colleagues in a recent AERO publication, Supporting students significantly behind in literacy and numeracy.

Nancy Young's Ladder of Reading and Writing is a helpful infographic for illustrating just how few children achieve success with minimally guided reading instruction. 

Balanced literacy is not compatible with RTI and MTSS, as they require evidence-based explicit teaching, with robust progress monitoring (i.e., not Running Records) and intervention (i.e., not Reading Recovery).

No child should ever have to recover from their initial reading instruction.

It takes four times as many resources to resolve a literacy problem by Year 4 than it would have taken in Year 1.

This point has also been made by a number of reading scientists, including by Dr Kerry Hempenstall in the paper linked to above. If by year 4, the multiplier is 4, what does it balloon to in Year 7, or Year 10? How do we choose between psychological supports for the emotional and behavioural sequelae of academic struggle on the one hand, and intervention supports targeting weak reading subskills on the other?

Why would we teach reading in ways that are known to promote high rates of failure? This is unconscionable in 2023.

There is no scientific evidence that supports balanced literacy and its key elements as a preferred reading instruction approach.

This is probably the most extraordinary “achievement” of those who promote balanced literacy. In spite of the fact that balanced literacy has not been shown through rigorous, empirical research to be the optimal way to produce reading success at a population level, it has been assigned and allowed to retain “untouchable” status.

In Australia, the community is protected against rogue health practitioners who want to introduce new medicines and devices through the oversight of the Therapeutic Goods Administration. In the US the Food and Drug Administration fulfils this role.

Where is the regulatory authority that protects children from non-evidence-based approaches to reading instruction? How are universities held to account for perpetuating these, year in, year out, in initial teacher education degrees (and charging for the privilege)?   

It is not the job of parents to teach children to read, write and spell.

This is a pernicious and damaging spin designed to shift responsibility from the education sector to the home. I have blogged in detail about this here. Suffice to say, we want parents to read to their children and support their language and literacy development wherever they can, if they can.

Reading to children doesn't turn them into readers however, any more than playing classical music to them turns them into pianists.

Balanced literacy has de-professionalised teachers by abdicating responsibility for ensuring teachers are classroom-ready as reading teachers, and then playing the parent-blame joker when its efforts fail children in the long tail of under-achievement.

Reading instruction needs to be informed by neuroscience, e.g., the work of Professor Stanislas Dehaene.

Professor Stanislas Dehaene’s work in recent years has been instrumental in reinforcing evidence derived from cognitive psychology research and classroom studies concerning optimal reading instruction. Most notably, it assists us to understand how high-quality initial reading instruction helps to transform our evolutionarily-derived language brains so that they can become reading brains.

Balanced literacy is silent on this.

 

Balanced literacy epitomises the golden mean fallacy or “argument to moderation”: the idea that when views on a topic are polarised, there must be a logical “sweet spot” in the middle where we all need to meet and strike a compromise. We have tried the sweet spot experiment in reading instruction, and it didn’t work. We can’t unsee the population-level data associated with the failed balanced literacy experiment. Teachers are haunted by the faces of children who they can’t forget; the ones left behind by balanced literacy’s known but brushed-over and forgiven shortcomings.

In response to an audience question about how New Zealand can overhaul its approach to reading instruction and support, given the weighty sentimental attachment to the Clay legacy, Emily Hanford made an incisive observation: New Zealand is about the size of a small US state and has one national government – no states and territories to wrangle to the table, as exist in Australia and the US.

I would add to this that New Zealand has already taken reading policy and practice to scale at a national level, albeit history records that this happened to be based on a then questioned, and now debunked theory of reading and reading support.

There is an opportunity now for New Zealand to re-write its reading instruction history and lead English-speaking countries out of the balanced literacy dark age.  

Now wouldn’t that be a spectacular way for a small nation to truly punch above its weight on the global stage?

(C) Pamela Snow (2023)

21 comments:

  1. Thank you. I was fortunate to listen to your presentation in Christchurch, and as an assessor of Specific Learning Disorders and Learning Support Specialist, I see many students who continue to be failed by NZ's approach to literacy instruction. I loved your suggestion to engage school leaders in conversation about structured literacy, "How satisfied are you with your data?" (or similar wording)...BUT unfortunately data gathering, AND reporting of achievement, at primary school is an optional exercise in NZ (unbelievable but true). So conveniently, what you don't measure, you don't need to manage AND you can't be held accountable for!

    ReplyDelete
  2. Every single word! As a principal in a high transience school this hits where it needs to. We can not continue to leverage our high achievers with our most vulnerable cohorts. A high transience system must have low variance curriculum and teaching.

    ReplyDelete
  3. Important points and a fantastic vision for New Zealand.

    ReplyDelete
  4. As an Australian teacher who was involved in transitioning my Qld school to structured literacy approaches, it is staggering to see how the NZ education system is so committed to continuing with the balanced literacy approach. I’m currently doing relief teaching in Auckland and no school I work in is engaged in the conversation about structured literacy. I hope this changes soon.

    ReplyDelete
  5. You hit the nail on the head when you said "national cognitive dissonance"! Quite a bit of individual cognitive dissonance too... some people just can't let go of the familiar, even in the face of resounding evidence to the contrary. How can we make the SOR as superficially enticing as the folksy promises of Balanced Literacy? Perhaps it's a marketing problem?

    ReplyDelete

  6. Your comment,
    ‘We can acquire a reading brain, but most of us need exposure to high quality instruction in the early years of school in order to do so.’
    is in contradiction to this blog - the figures of those that do learn to read as opposed to those that don’t, or not proficiently, confuse your argument. if the BL, whatever that is, doesn’t work, then just how have the vast majority learnt to read, when supposedly most of us ‘need high quality instruction to do so?

    With all the schools that are now doing SOR (and have done for a number of years) in Australia and all the SOR PD undertaken over the past years (many many thousands of schools and teachers is a figure that has been quoted) why weren’t NAPLAN scores in Australia just a wee bit, or a lot better for 2023 - rather than worse?

    Perhaps teacher educators did/do look at the evidence and is the reason teachers have been able to provide the high quality instruction for the majority to succeed. But perhaps we need to spend time and effort in many, many different ways, including more intense intervention to cater for the needs of those that do struggle to acquire literacy skills. And it is just not all phonics as many remedial/intervention phonics programs have now proven.

    ReplyDelete
  7. Hello Anonymous
    I'm not 100% sure I follow your argument, but I will respond as best I can. Language and reading skills sit on a continuum of ability, so it is to be expected that some children will catch-on to reading more readily than others will - for a range of reasons (genetic, environmental, experiential, individual motivation, etc - some of which are quite difficult to quantify). My point is not that BL "doesn't work" - it is that it is not successful with enough students - as per Nancy Young's Ladder of Reading and Writing (see link in row 6 of the table above).

    You refer to "all the schools that are now doing SOR in Australia" but the reality is that we have no idea how many schools are on a SoR journey, how far advanced they are, the fidelity of their implementation, and whether or not the SoR is being used by some schools to re-badge what is essentially business-as-usual BL, with a few add-ons.

    NAPLAN is a macro-level outcome, but we do not have a detailed picture of the inputs. What we do know is that devolved responsibility to individual schools results in high levels of variability in reading instruction, and, as I have noted previously, variability in any system is the enemy of quality.

    The problem with the term "majority" is that it can encompass anything from 51% to 99%, but at the moment, is nowhere near 99%.

    And finally, I am not aware of anyone who is claiming that phonics is "the answer" but high quality decoding skills are a non-negotiable for reading success, and this includes into the secondary years, where the nature of the task becomes more complex, with students having to read and understand unfamiliar polysyllabic, multi-morphemic words across the curriculum.

    ReplyDelete
  8. Years ago, I was fortunate to be at an International Reading Association which is now ILA, when there was a heated discussion over whole language and phonics. All of the “experts” were there. It is surprising to me now to hear the rationale as to why the name Balanced Literacy was chosen. It was not a negative then. It was a solution in order to give students quality literature and the decoding skills to understand and use the sound system. It is my through my training that if a teacher understands the pedagogy and methodology of reading, they will provide a continuum that consist of explicit instruction within a systematic approach that students will understand, use and become proficient readers. Are we really about students? This makes me sad. So much of this talk will never reach teachers so that they can make a difference in the lives of children. This is where the real work begins.

    ReplyDelete
  9. Hello Anonymous
    In my NZ keynotes, I addressed the fallacy of the "golden mean" - the idea that when there's polarised views on a topic, simply meeting in the middle is the appropriate solution. I didn't include this idea in the current blogpost, but you can read about it in this open access publication: http://www.onlinedigeditions.com/publication/?i=655062&article_id=3634779&view=articleBrowser&ver=html5

    The problem, I think, is that BL has come to be synonymous with "choose your own adventure" as to how reading is taught, regarding structure and sequencing of content and skills. High variability and high quality are not compatible with each other.

    I agree that teacher understanding is key, but there is a large body of evidence (linked to in my post above) indicating that teacher knowledge is weak in this area, due to known gaps in initial teacher education.

    I'm all about students (and teachers). That's why I write a blog that seeks to promote knowledge translation for teachers. They are the ones on the ground, doing life-changing work for their students every day.

    ReplyDelete
  10. NZ born, bred and proud.

    I find this virulent attempt to degrade all the awesome work of thousands of NZ teachers who have taught millions to read and write using Clay’s work and a balanced literacy process (which includes phonics), degrading of both schools and hard-working teachers.

    If you truly wanted change in your position and with your connections you would be fighting with education ministers and governments to fund Government schools to adequately provide for all kids, to provide specialised reading clinics for those that need more time and repetition to acquire reading and writing, and teachers and classrooms for those with special needs to develop their literacy acquisition. It would be great if schools were like hospitals providing high-quality specialist care, especially for those in low SE areas where many and varied SE issues and language barriers are holding back many learners.

    To imply that academics do not have knowledge and understanding of decades of cognitive psychology research on the nature of the reading process is short of disgraceful. This then implies that teachers have no understanding of how we learn. There are awesome, amazing teachers out there using a range of teaching strategies and getting outstanding results and have done so for decades!

    This blog I feel is an attempt to undermine all the work of Clay, while the ‘high quality’ SoR programs I have seen incorporate her strategies and activities. It feels for me like an attempt by Australian brand-building academics and American storytelling journalists to simplify the massive social and economic problems of today that are affecting teaching, literacy and learning - by blaming an amazing educator.

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Well said, I totally agree. Ann, USA

      Delete
  11. As an assessor of Specific Learning Disorders, teacher and provider of learning support in NZ, I see the legacy of Clay's work and that of our failed literacy approach in the desperate students I meet everyday. The problems have been identified over and over by the few unblinkered academics in NZ, such as Prof. James Chapman...yet ignored by our Ministry of Education. I don't know of the outstanding results that you refer to....our NMSSA 2019 data show that only 56% of Year 8 students are meeting curriculum expectations in reading, and only 35% in writing. The most recent Progress in International Reading Literacy Study (PIRLS) showed that our reading scores had fallen to the lowest since this study began (i.e. literacy skills are now lower than they were 20 years ago). Perhaps it is time to take those blinkers off and look at the research from academics such as Prof Pam Snow, and other leading scientists from around the rest of the world. We owe the best to our students, not holding onto some romantic and defunct ideology of yesteryear.

    ReplyDelete
  12. Unfortunately increased expenditure on education (in Australia at least) has not translated into improved student outcomes, so we have to entertain the possibility that the nature of the instruction is where solutions lie, not in budgets alone. Of course this is ultimately empowering to teachers, because they are the ones who determine how children's time is spent in the classroom. There's a report about recent Productivity Commission findings about this at this link: https://www.theguardian.com/australia-news/2016/sep/06/more-education-spending-not-raising-standards-productivity-commission-says

    ReplyDelete
  13. It is not the amount of money, but how ithe money is used. I am in Australia and millions of dollars have been put into DI, phonics and literacy programs for our Indigenous schools over the past decade without success.
    There is way more complexity to addressing failure in literacy than phonics and DI. BL has been around for decades with pretty consistent results until the last decade when many more and different social problems have developed and need to be addressed . The growth in ESL learners, navigating the now extraordinary numbers of children in childcare entering school, the digital world effect on early learning, SE and emotional crisis,
    I am not a RR fan, but a simplistic lens on a complex problem is not going to solve problems but exacerbate them.

    Rhetoric such as this blog with finger pointing is set out to divide and conquer as the case has been in this debate for decades. We need to grow up, to highlight and respond to all the elements that are effecting teaching, rather than believe there is one simple answer.
    Yes to promoting explicit phonics teaching as essential and nonnegotiable elements of a balanced ‘teaching’ process, but no to ignoring what elements have worked and worked well.

    ReplyDelete
  14. "you would be fighting with education ministers and governments to fund Government schools to adequately provide for all kids, to provide specialised reading clinics for those that need more time and repetition to acquire reading and writing, and teachers and classrooms for those with special needs to develop their literacy acquisition. It would be great if schools were like hospitals providing high-quality specialist care"

    Two points:

    1) Has it occurred to you that if there weren't so many instructional casualties, we wouldn't need "specialized reading clinics"?

    2) If schools were like hospitals, they would all be following the same evidence-based practices, which is currently not the case. Does hospital care vary dramatically depending on the doctor on duty?

    ReplyDelete
  15. Hello Harriet
    Many of the ‘casualties’ as you call them are because of the lack of teaching time for catch-up for those who need more instruction.
    If you have taught whole classes you know there will be differences in learning rates. It could be because age, attendance, speech language barriers, processing difficulties, trauma etc. Hence, with the very same teacher and lessons we have different outcomes, even when using the highest quality SoR programs.

    More time and specific intervention is needed for these students. It is not that some students can’t do, it is they need more time. So providing catch up teaching time with practice time gives these students the opportunity of success,

    With even more crowded curriculums over the years, catch-up time for specific students, giving them the intervention and practice they need is near impossible. This is where funding and lots of funding needs to be targeted.

    We know all students don’t learn at the same rate or with the same attention rate.. But this is ignored and the burden is taken on by the classroom teacher. Your teaching doesn’t work.

    When these students are given one-on-one remediation we see success. We need to cater more thoroughly for those that take longer to learn. This will level the playing field.

    So, as in your hospital analogy of following the science, all patients don’t respond the same to the same treatment, some need more time, some need different treatments, some need specialist help and some develop other issues or die given the same treatment. And yes, I am sure we have all had doctors who diagnose differently and treat symptoms differently.

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. "Many of the ‘casualties’ as you call them are because of the lack of teaching time for catch-up for those who need more instruction.
      If you have taught whole classes you know there will be differences in learning rates. It could be because age, attendance, speech language barriers, processing difficulties, trauma etc. Hence, with the very same teacher and lessons we have different outcomes, even when using the highest quality SoR programs."

      Here's what I have learned over two decades of teaching kindergarten and first grade whole class and working with small groups of students as a reading specialist. The programs based on methods promoted by Marie Clay do not set students up for success, and I have to spend considerable time undoing the damage these methods and materials cause. No one is saying that SoR is a panacea for all reading problems. What we are saying is that it provides a baseline of solid foundational skill instruction so that 'unexpected failure' experienced by some students can lead us to explore other causes like dyslexia.

      Delete
    2. Harriett6 September 2023 at 11:08
      Reading Recovery teaching does not ignore 'unexpected failure' experienced by some students and RR has also led us to explore other causes like dyslexia. SoR lobby group activists say that they cater for dyslexia and it will not occur under their teaching. Still stand by the facts, It is interesting more failures now in literacy under SoR than before.

      Delete
    3. I'm not quite sure what facts you're standing by and how you've determined that there are "more failures now in literacy under SoR than before." Here are the facts I stand by:

      Getting Reading Right: On Truths, Truce, and Trust
      https://pamelasnow.blogspot.com/2023/01/guest-post-getting-reading-right-on.html

      Delete
  16. Arohamai Tena koe Talofa Kia Ora ra Pamela Our tamariki/ children are not suffering from developmental disorders or any other cognitive or langurs delay so pleas do not seek to impose approaches designed for children in your specialised field on all out children It is unethical behaviour Our children and millions of others in the world are growing up bilingual and need to be biliterate I do not believe you understand about these fields of childhood at all so Nor do they deserved to be used in your political campaigns to gain power and control over our entire education system Please stay in your own org field aka lane
    Mauri ora

    ReplyDelete
  17. You’re absolutely right Anonymous. Low literacy rates in NZ are not due to developmental disabilities. They are due to poor instruction, auspiced by universities and the education ministry. It’s great to see this announcement from the NZ National Party ahead of your forthcoming election:
    https://www.1news.co.nz/2023/09/08/nats-vow-to-shake-up-how-kiwi-kids-learn-to-read/

    ReplyDelete