It
is sometimes tempting, in the field of early literacy instruction, to think
that progress has completely stalled, and any real hope of lifting literacy
levels in Australia (and similar western nations) has slipped through our
fingers. So, to start with the good news: it is pleasing that we seem to have some
level of agreement that there are many pillars of early literacy that require
focus in the first three years of school. Researchers and practitioners at
pretty much any point on the “Whole-Language – Phonics” continuum (and I do believe
it is a continuum rather than a dichotomy) affirm the importance of the so-called “Big Five”
(phonics, phonemic awareness, vocabulary, comprehensions, and fluency). That said, an Australian education professor last year appeared to challenge this consensus, going so far, in fact, as to argue
that a focus “solely” on the Big Five elements could be “damaging” to some
children. In the absence, however, of any evidence to support the astonishing
contention that a focus on these elements does actual harm to children’s
reading progress, I think we can assume that they constitute a good and widely
agreed-upon basis for classroom reading instruction.
However, it is not enough to just put all the elements
into a metaphorical blender and hope for the best. In this sense, so-called
“Balanced Literacy” (BL) might be better termed “Blended Literacy” – all of the
elements outlined above are in there somewhere, but the literature describing
them in the context of BL does not advocate that they are treated in a systematic, sequential way.
Perhaps this stems from a lack of an agreed definition of BL.
In my search of the literature, I found (e.g.
see Rasinski & Padak, 2004) various and varied references to a “compromise position”
between Whole Language and phonics, the importance of “multiple literacies”, BL
as a “radical middle”, the adoption of “principled pluralism”, a “new consensus
in literacy education”, and statements such as
“…. balanced literacy becomes a recognition that language
and literacy theories and practices need to be ‘answerable to concrete others’" (Heydon et al., 2005, p. 313).
Now, I do not know exactly what “concrete others” means, but
if it is another way of talking about accountability, then I am on board with
it.
Wren (2001) observed that “A balanced approach could be
generically described as ‘mixing some Phonics with Whole Language,’ but how
this is accomplished in any particular classroom is unclear” (p. 4). Another BL
researcher (Mermelstein, 2006) further highlighted this lack of definitional
clarity thus:
“Often when I visit schools and ask teachers to describe
their curriculum to me, they’ll say that they do “balanced literacy.” I often
ask them what they mean by “balanced.” The answers I get are varied. Their
varied answers don’t surprise me because as I worked on this article I
discovered a variety of views as to what the word balanced in “balanced
literacy” actually means”.
So - one basic problem with BL is its lack of a robust,
agreed-upon definition.
I wonder then, how many beginning teachers would find this
take on BL of practical value when they enter an early years’ classroom:
“The version of balanced literacy that we espouse …. cautions
educators about the slipperiness of subjectivities, power relations, and the
inability of an abstract theory or practice to adequately control, predict, or
define the needs of a classroom of students and as such insists that all theory
and practice be situated within the relationship between teacher, student,
time, and place” (Heydony, Hibbert & Iannacci, 2004, p. 313). If anyone can
tell me what this actually means in relation
to the task of teaching children to read, I am all ears.
Another notable absence in the BL literature (this 2006 piece by Starrett comes close though and please
update me if I have missed something), is discussion of the fact that there are
different approaches to phonics instruction, and they are neither
synonymous nor inter-changeable. Analytic, incidental, and embedded phonics
align more with Whole Language (whole-to-part analysis) than to approaches such
as systematic synthetic phonics that emphasise part-to-whole analysis for
beginning readers (see this link for more information).
In fact, the frequent use of the word “eclectic” in the BL literature is the big clue as to what is really
going on in many early years’ classrooms. Eclectic has a meaning that is
diametrically opposed to systematic. This eclecticism, when coupled with
Kenneth Goodman-esque “teachers should be left alone to decide what’s best for
children in their classroom” means that BL is really an Alice-in-Wonderland
term that can mean pretty much exactly whatever the user wants it to mean. As
has been noted elsewhere (Bingham & Hall-Kenyon 2013; Bowen & Snow, 2017) this creates one of the challenging (some
might even say clever) aspects of BL from a research perspective – how do we
evaluate something that is likely to look very different from one early years’
classroom to the next? Normally, fidelity of delivery is a cornerstone of
intervention evaluation, but we can’t establish fidelity if the intervention
itself is, by design, open to interpretation by those who implement it.
All of this would, of course, be a moot point if we were
doing a better job of getting more children across the bridge to literacy in
the first three years of school. But we’re not.
My read of the BL literature also highlights a confusing and
disproportionate emphasis in many BL publications on “higher-order” processes
(synthesis, analysis, metacognition), while simultaneously overlooking the
lower-order skills that need to be in place (i.e. need to be taught) to free up cognitive resources
in order to support such processes. Take for example, this 2008 paper, accessible via the Australian Literacy Educators’ Association (ALEA) website. Its author (Cozmescu) refers to the use of a “bricolage" of classroom practices,
but makes no reference to phonics instruction as a teaching approach,
despite referring to decoding as a strategy that children might use when
participating in activities that respond to the question “How will I ensure
that this week’s literacy program will include all of my students”? How indeed.
And how do children call on a strategy such as decoding, if they have not been
taught it?
It is also interesting to read the views of proponents of BL
on its historical roots. For example, Manset-Williamson, and Nelson (2005, p.
59) observed that “Influential within the debate over explicitness of strategy
instruction are Fountas and Pinnell
(1996), who have argued that strategies cannot be directly taught. Instead,
they propose that teachers provide rich literature experiences for students so
that reading strategies can be naturally constructed with teacher support, but
not explicit instruction”.
Did you catch that? “Strategies
cannot be directly taught”. Many readers will be aware of course, that
Fountas and Pinnell are the authors of the hugely popular and widely used (in
Australia at least) levelled readers based around predictable text that
encourage guessing rather than decoding as a first-line approach for beginning
readers. Their advice flies in the face not only of decades of cognitive
psychology research, and the findings of three national inquiries into the
teaching of literacy (USA, Australia, and UK), but ironically is also at odds
with published statements by some other proponents of BL. For example,
Rivalland (2000, p. 2), referred to “Explicit instruction in code-breaking
techniques, which include phonological awareness, letter recognition,
letter-sound correspondences and sight word recognition” - in spite of the fact
that Fountas and Pinnell said that reading strategies cannot be directly
taught. Notably, Rivalland still stops short of reference to initial
systematic, synthetic phonics as the preferred approach to teaching decoding
skills, but I think readers can see just how confusing and contradictory the BL landscape is.
Offering eclectic, only partially targeted instructional
approaches as a means of meeting the diverse needs of early learners seems an
odd vehicle for a pedagogical approach that has the word “balance” in its name.
It’s somewhat akin to using a butter knife to slice a piece of rump steak: it
superficially looks like an implement
that might be fit for purpose, but in
practice offers only a blunt interface with the task at hand.
Thankfully the Five from Five project offers a means of redressing some of the gaps and inconsistencies
outlined above. It is based on the agreed evidence that children need teachers
who can draw together processes in phonics, phonemic awareness, vocabulary
development, comprehension, and reading fluency, but value-adds these by
positioning systematic synthetic phonics instruction as a first-line approach,
and provides resources to teachers and parents to support this. This is an important refinement that is sorely lacking in
current conceptualisations of BL. It also neatly gets past the problem of
phonics being the only one of the Big Five that needs to “justify” its
existence as a pedagogical pillar.
There is perhaps a perverse reward for lack of systematic
early reading teaching in the fact that many children will cross the bridge to
literacy almost irrespective of the pedagogical focus in their classroom. Such
children enter school with a range of bio-psycho-social advantages that set
them up for success. This position was well-summarised by Konza, who observed (2014, p. 160)
"It is true that some children readily acquire the skills of independent reading without highly explicit teaching, but if balanced is interpreted as offering all children only an embedded rather than an explicit approach to phonics instruction, those most in need will be further disadvantaged".
So we cannot expect to see improvements in the performance of children in the tail of the achievement curve unless and until teachers are equipped to teach children who start from behind and require explicit, systematic teaching if they are to have any chance of catching up, let alone engaging with the higher-order language-based literacy activities that await them across the curriculum.
"It is true that some children readily acquire the skills of independent reading without highly explicit teaching, but if balanced is interpreted as offering all children only an embedded rather than an explicit approach to phonics instruction, those most in need will be further disadvantaged".
So we cannot expect to see improvements in the performance of children in the tail of the achievement curve unless and until teachers are equipped to teach children who start from behind and require explicit, systematic teaching if they are to have any chance of catching up, let alone engaging with the higher-order language-based literacy activities that await them across the curriculum.
Compromise is a
wonderful thing, in its place. But we should not be compromising on translating
scientific evidence into classroom practice. It is not enough to offer children
(especially those starting from behind) an eclectic,
bricolaged, blended literacy program that fails for want of a simple early
emphasis on explicit and systematic synthetic phonics instruction, and then to
classify such children as “special needs” when they inevitably and completely
predictably display low literacy skills by mid primary school years. This is
simply amounts to another way of describing Dr. Reid Lyon’s “instructional casualties”.
That’s not fair to teachers or students, and it’s most
certainly not what I call "balanced".
POSTSCRIPT, October 2019: I recommend the explanation at this link (courtesy of Education Week) as to how BL and effective phonics instruction differ from each other.
References
References
Bingham, G.E. & Hall-Kenyon, K.M. (2013). Examining
teachers’ beliefs about and implementation of a balanced literacy framework. Journal of Research in Reading, 36(1), 14-28.
Bowen, C. & Snow, P. (2017). Making Sense of Interventions for Children with Developmental
Disorders. A Guide for Parents and Professionals. Croydon, UK: J&R
Press http://www.jr-press.co.uk/making-sense-of-interventions-for-childrens-developmental-disorders.html
Cozmescu, H. (2008). Thinking
Balanced Literacy Planning https://www.alea.edu.au/documents/item/102
Heydon, R., Hibbert, K. & Iannacci. L. (2005).
Strategies to support balanced literacy approaches in pre- and inservice
teacher education. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 48(4), 312-319.
Konza, D. (2014). Teaching reading: Why the "Fab Five" should be "the Big Six". Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 39(12), 153-169.
Manset-Williamson, G. & Nelson, J.M. (2005). Balanced, strategic reading instruction for upper-elementary and middle school students with reading disabilities: A comparative study of two approaches. Learning Disability Quarterly, 28(1),59-74.
Konza, D. (2014). Teaching reading: Why the "Fab Five" should be "the Big Six". Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 39(12), 153-169.
Manset-Williamson, G. & Nelson, J.M. (2005). Balanced, strategic reading instruction for upper-elementary and middle school students with reading disabilities: A comparative study of two approaches. Learning Disability Quarterly, 28(1),59-74.
Mermelstein, L. (2006). The components of Balanced Literacy.
What does Balanced Literacy actually mean? https://www.education.com/pdf/components-balanced-literacy/
Rasinski, T. & Padak, N. (2004). Beyond consensus—beyond
balance: Toward a comprehensive literacy curriculum. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 20(1), 91-102
Rivalland, J. (2000). Finding a balance for the year 2000
and beyond. Newsletter of the Australian
Literacy Educators' Association, February. https://www.alea.edu.au/documents/item/62
Wren, S. (2001). What does a “Balanced Literacy Approach” mean? http://catalogue.nla.gov.au/Record/5686259
(C) Pamela Snow, 2017
(C) Pamela Snow, 2017
Right on! Nobody would go for "unbalanced illiteracy," but "balanced literacy" is just the other side of the empty term. The antidote to the empty terminology is the Alphabetic Code [Phonics] Screening Check that you have a major role in implementing in Au.
ReplyDeleteI agree Dick - it is a whitewash term, rendering it pretty meaningless, though the use of the word "balanced" has a superficial appeal of course.
ReplyDeleteHi Pamela. Thank you for this article. Is there any evidence for the value of a BL approach for students (children or adults) with an intellectual impairment? I am a practitioner of Orton Gillingham based methods for children with LDs but in looking around for information on how to teach adults with an II, I keep running into BL.
ReplyDeleteHi Jacqui
ReplyDeleteFirst up, my apologies for missing your comment. There's a lot of spam that finds its way through and I don't always filter as well as I should. I am not aware of any specific research on BL for children with intellectual disability. The key problem with doing such research would of course be coming up with a robust operational definition of what BL actually is. We can't test a method on any population if we can't first agree on what the method looks like.
Thank you for referencing this piece as part of the renewed discussion over balanced literacy. It really does provide comprehensive context for BL. Very helpful!
ReplyDeleteThank you Pamela from an SLP working in state education in Aus. Always grateful for your work.
ReplyDelete