Professor Dianne Ravitch, who describes herself as “a historian of education and Research
Professor of Education at New York University” has written a
brief blog-post this week headlined “There is no science of reading”. Her blog-post is an endorsement of the US
National Education Policy Center (NEPC) March 20 Policy Statement on the Science of Reading.
National Education Policy Center (NEPC) March 20 Policy Statement on the Science of Reading.
The inescapable problem for the NEPC, Professor Ravitch and those who sing from the same songbook, is that there is a science of reading, in the same way
that there is a science of memory, a science of cognition, a science of
perception, a science of language, and a science of learning. And the science of reading is not going away.
In all of these fields (as indeed, in the science of reading
field), scientific endeavours are ongoing, building incrementally on knowledge
which is released via academics to practitioners. Practitioners then apply it,
to be part of the knowledge translation loop that results in ongoing refinements
of theories and their implications for policy and practice.
However, the science of reading has been the Cinderella of knowledge
translation because of the resistance of some influential players to adopting
the scientific method and changing their messaging as shifts in evidence occur.
If we left debate about reading instruction solely in the
hands of Whole Language and Balanced Literacy advocates, it’s difficult to see
how the discourse would have shifted at all in recent decades. What, for example,
are most pre-service teachers learning in 2020, that is materially
different from what they were learning in 2010? Or 2000? Or even earlier?
If we interrogate Balanced Literacy practices employed in
the late 1990s what would we find?
- An emphasis on Three Cueing (Multi-Cueing) that encourages guessing rather than decoding;
- banks of de-contextualised sight words that have no scope and sequence with respect to their phoneme-grapheme correspondences;
- reliance on predictable levelled readers;
- reluctance to apply phonics-based instruction of any kind, but begrudging acceptance of analytic phonics as a final resort;
- advice to parents that they should discourage children from decoding through the word, because this is not true engagement with text;
- progress monitoring tools such as Running Records rather than psychometrically valid alternatives.
And if we interrogate Balanced Literacy practices 30 years
later in 2020, is anything different?
Back to the future.
Telling teachers that
The
truth is that there is no settled science of reading. The research on reading
and teaching reading is abundant, but it is diverse and always in a state of
change.
is designed to foster knowledge translation paralysis and
keep teachers in the dark with respect to shifts in understanding that occur in
research circles about approaches that are optimal for all children, not merely for
those who will likely succeed, irrespective of their backgrounds and the pedagogical
rigour to which they are exposed.
When children do not succeed in Balanced Literacy classrooms
the next step is to make blanket calls for better school funding, which understandably
chimes with most teachers. Who would not want more money for their school? But
there is a fixed cost to the tax-payer of having a teacher in a classroom.
Would it not be better use of everyone’s resources for that teacher to be an
expert on the current sate of play in the science of reading so s/he can apply
this, in the tradition of scientist-practitioner that we promote in other
fields such as medicine and psychology?
Ravitch also quotes and endorses the NEPC statement that:
This
key idea of a “balanced literacy” approach stresses the importance of phonics,
authentic reading, and teachers who can teach reading using a full toolbox of
instructional approaches and understandings. It is strongly supported in the
scholarly community and is grounded in a large research base.
I have written previously about the self-described instructional
bricolage that is Balanced Literacy. Having everything in the mix does not
constitute systematic instruction by knowledgeable teachers. And just who is
the “scholarly community” that is being swept up into one convenient collective
bundle by this generalisation? I am part of the scholarly community and I for
one, did not sign up to have my views appropriated in this way.
Ravitch also endorses the NEPC argument that
Education
legislation should address guiding concepts while avoiding prescriptions that
will tie the hands of professional educators.
This is just another way of saying “let a thousand flowers
bloom”, or “choose your own adventure” and is what sustains this ongoing mess.
If Balanced Literacy was a success-story, why are such large proportions of
children in English-speaking industrialised nations not reaching their reading
(and academic) potential? Where will the jobs be for these future unskilled workers
as our economies replace unskilled roles with artificial intelligence and other
forms of technology?
Posts such as this recent one by Ravitch (and the NEPC policy statement on which it is based) are a blatant
attempt to slow the march of science and in particular, to retard its transfer
into the classroom, via transmission to pre-service teachers. Many in education
bemoan what is sometimes seen as the low professional standing of teaching as a
profession. There is no more sure way of keeping education in the professional
esteem shadows than preaching flat-earth mis-truths such as “there is no
science of reading”.
Some years ago, in Australia, there was a proposal prior
to a federal election, to bring in a goods and services (consumption) tax. Do
you know how this proposal was rolled? By a clever political slogan: “If you
don’t understand it, don’t vote for it”.
Exactly the same meme is being plied at teachers with
respect to the science of reading.
Unfortunately, there is abundant evidence
that teachers in the main, are not exposed to the science of reading in their
pre-service years (see references
at this link). It is not surprising, therefore that many do not have an
explicit grasp of such linguistic concepts as phonemes, morphemes, graphemes, syllables,
phonemic awareness, phoneme-grapheme correspondences, digraphs, trigraphs,
schwa vowels, and the need to teach these explicitly, with a scope and sequence
in play.
But perhaps there’s another group who don’t understand
it and so don’t vote for it: those in the academy and government who are stuck
in a 1990s vision of what they would like the process of reading to be,
rather than a 2020 science-informed position about its complexity and optimal
ways of unlocking this for students.
The only reason that we are not closer to a consensus on what constitutes the science of reading (what the reading process is and optimal ways of teaching it) is the deliberate obfuscation and anti-science rhetoric of some in pre-service teacher education and influential policy roles.
The only reason that we are not closer to a consensus on what constitutes the science of reading (what the reading process is and optimal ways of teaching it) is the deliberate obfuscation and anti-science rhetoric of some in pre-service teacher education and influential policy roles.