It’s a strange thing, that you can have your doubts about a person or an idea, and sometimes these doubts just lurk in the back of your mind for a long time, surfacing unexpectedly during informal conversations, or in the question-time after a seminar or conference presentation. Then one day, an off-hand comment or innocent question brings a moment of crystalline clarity. So it was for me a couple of years ago when I had finished giving a one-hour presentation about early oral language competence and developmental trajectories to an audience of about 200 primary school teachers, and someone asked me a question about dyslexia. To my surprise (and I am sure to that of many in the audience, not to mention the organisers), I found myself answering “I’m sorry, I have no idea what dyslexia is”.
I immediately sensed the unease in the room of course, but
went on to explain that it’s a term that (in Australia at least) has never acquired
a robust, ring-fenced meaning accompanied by tight and transparent diagnostic
criteria. Rather, it is a term that is applied, sometimes, by some
professionals, to some children, who
have various difficulties with reading, whether with decoding, comprehension,
fluency, or a combination there-of.
I was reminded of this experience recently, when I heard
about a forthcoming text, by Professors Julian (Joe) Elliott
of Durham University and Elena
Grigorenko, of Yale University, Connecticut “The Dyslexia Debate”. I have pre-ordered a copy and am looking
forward to understanding their arguments against the use of the term dyslexia,
particularly given that Professor Elliot is in the UK, where the term has had a
much stronger uptake than here in Australia. You can listen to some of
Professor Elliot’s thoughts here.
So, if we were to find three 8 year-olds in a Grade 2
classroom whose reading skills are say, 12 - 18 months below expected levels, would
we say they all have dyslexia? If so,
on what basis? What would we need to exclude in order for diagnostic criteria
to be met? Can you have dyslexia if you have not been exposed to appropriate
instructional techniques in the first three years of school? If not, what
should we call your reading problems? “Reading
problems”? Can you have dyslexia if you also have a mild intellectual
disability? Or does a diagnosis of ID “trump” a diagnosis of dyslexia and
automatically account for the child’s reading problems?
I went to school with a girl who had difficulties with
writing, spelling and verbal expression that seemed, even to other students, to
be disproportionate to her otherwise robust intellectual skills. I remember her
explaining to us that she had dyslexia – “A thing between my eyes and my brain
that makes it impossible to read words properly”. At the time of course, I visualised a physical
obstruction of some sort. Years later, when I enrolled in a four-year Speech Pathology
degree, I expected that I would finally learn all about this thing between the
eyes and the brain that impeded my friend’s academic progress.
But no. I did learn many Latin and Greek roots and affixes
to describe a number of speech and language disturbances, but dyslexia rated
barely a mention. Its close relative alexia
was certainly covered in the context of (acquired) communication disorders of neurological
origin, most notably certain left hemisphere lesions after stroke. Acquired
alexia is often seen in people with aphasia and represents a breakdown in
reading and writing a language with which the patient was familiar prior to
their neurological event.
However my growing knowledge of a/dysphasia, alexia,
a/dyspraxia, an/dysarthria etc was not augmented by a specific understanding of
the term “dyslexia”. It seemed that I had come no further than my conversation
with my school friend ten years earlier. Granted, it was a term I heard used by
parents and in the media, but it was not part of my own professional lexicon.
Let me stress though, that I had no doubt then (or now) that
significant numbers of children, for a variety of reasons, have reading difficulties/disorders. Sometimes these
children were described as having learning disabilities, learning difficulties, or learning disorders,
or (more ideally I believe) as having language-learning
disorders, reflecting the heavy reliance of reading skills on underlying
oral language capacities. Of course many
of these children also have writing and spelling difficulties. These problems combine to wreak havoc in the lives of affected children, academically and psychologically and have significant implications at a wider population health level across the life-span. They are in no way to be taken lightly.
I became aware over the years though, that dyslexia is a
term that is widely used in the UK, where it seems to have gained significant traction
as a diagnostic label, both in childhood and across the lifespan. However this
knowledge has done little to unpack my continued confusion about what dyslexia
is, and what it isn’t.
Diagnostic
labels have all sorts of pros and cons.
On the up-side, they can help to explain otherwise baffling
collections of symptoms and signs, thus providing parents with a form of
psychoeducation that can be enormously beneficial in alleviating stress and
anxiety. They can head–off mis-guided and punitive responses by parents and
teachers and help a child’s educational needs to be better accommodated in the
classroom setting. In both children and
adults, a label such as dyslexia, which has a rather medical ring to it, also
helps to de-stigmatise sufferers – and let’s face it, there is considerable
shame attached to being unable to read and write as an adult. Some adults go to
excruciating lengths to conceal their difficulties from family, friends, and
employers, and who can blame them? And of course, a label such as dyslexia can,
in some jurisdictions, attract services, and what parent would not want to do
that?
But there is a problem in applying a diagnostic label that sounds specific when in fact the difficulties
it encompasses are often quite varied in their nature and severity and have
many different causes.
Unfortunately, we may have unwittingly created a two-tiered
landscape in which one person’s reading disorder is validated as being due to a
“medical problem” (dyslexia), where the next person just has a garden-variety “reading
problem” – perhaps reflecting their (perceived) laziness or lack of intellectual
aptitude.
The evidence-based interventions for children and adults with reading
difficulties are the same, irrespective of what they are called, so how is it
helpful to apply a formal label to some, and not to others?
Distancing
reading problems from instructional techniques
Another major reason that I don’t like the term dyslexia is
that it can unfortunately enable adults in the child’s world to abdicate
responsibility for the child’s reading skills (or lack there-of). If
appropriate instructional techniques (i.e. those based on synthetic phonics
approaches employing direct instruction) are not employed in the early years
and a child reaches Grade 3 not having achieved expected benchmarks, is that
child now functionally “dyslexic”? The great risk here of course is that
schools can give up on such children, as the child, rather than the instructional
environment, is seen as the problem.
In medicine, we refer to illnesses or other adverse outcomes
that result from medical mishaps as iatrogenic
– from the Greek “iatro” – physician
and “genic” – stemming from. We
should be careful, then, that we are not creating reading problems (dyslexia or
not) that might best be referred to as “edugenic”.
So, dyslexia – who are
you really?
Tweet
© Pamela Snow 2014
Thank you for this thoughtful and provocative post.I notice your reference to 'garden variety' reading problems. I'm sure you must already have been there but I find Keith Stanovich's 'Explaining the Differences between the Dyslexic and Garden-Variety Poor Readers very insightful, too.
ReplyDeleteYou are certainly right about the lengths to which people who can't read will go to try and disguise it. Some years ago a woman rang me to ask if I thought I could teach her husband to read. She was doing this without his knowledge and was desperate to help him. It transpired that whenever they went out for a meal, she would have to read the contents of the menu, ostensibly 'wondering aloud' what she would have to eat. This, of course, was a ruse to enable her husband to decide what he wanted to eat.
I asked her to talk to her husband and promised that I would test him free of charge if she could persuade him to come in. Sadly, I never heard from her again.
It's a great blog, Pamela! Keep up your wonderful work!
John
Thanks for your encouragement John, it is much appreciated. Stories such as the one you shared here are so sad, and so unnecessary. It's so unjust that some people's lives have such heavy shadows cast over them as a consequence of low reading skills. The shame attached to this for adults must be like a bag of rocks to carry every day - and of course this is entirely avoidable.
ReplyDeleteI do think the label of Dyslexia is very useful, and is supported by cognitive and fMRI research. There are a group of children who need much more intense reading instruction. My daughter (who is not dyslexic) was in a general ed classroom that was using a very good phonics program. I volunteered once a week for a year in her 1st grade classroom and helped the kids with their independent seat work while the teacher worked with 6 students at a time in the reading program. The top group was sailing through the program and bored out of their minds, the middle two groups seemed to finding the instruction just right, and all the kids in the bottom group were struggling. There was one boy in particular who couldn't read his seat work at all, but got all the answers easily when we volunteers read it to him. He needed a different intervention than the girl in the middle group who could read the passage accurately out loud but couldn't answer the comprehension questions, and a different intervention than the student who was new to English and was mainly struggling with vocabulary. Labeling each of their difficulties is the first step toward getting them the kind of help that they need. If we are going to have cutoffs for extra help, they should be very high ones. If gave targeted instruction to those in the lowest 25-30% instead of the lowest 5-10%, we would not have a literacy crisis in this country.
ReplyDelete