This article is my invited commentary on the July 2018 Phonics Debate, and has been reproduced here with permission from The Professional Educator, October 2018, special edition: The Great Literacy Debate (pp. 37-40)
The recent Phonics Debate (Sydney, July 31) has acted as a crucible in which long-held and deeply committed views and
antipathies have been exposed and stirred, possibly heralding a new chapter in
a long-running, corrosive debate about early reading instruction.
Against a background of a widening gulf in
Australia between the reading “haves” and “have nots”, there is cold comfort in
knowing that people on both sides of this debate place a premium on the
importance of early literacy attainment as a life-long asset. If this widening
gulf did not exist, there would be no “Reading Wars”. The Reading Wars draw their
oxygen, not from ongoing dissent, but from ongoing under-performance of
Australian children, particularly those who start from behind and are doomed by
a fatal mix of edu-nihilism and suboptimal instruction, to stay that way. There is also an increasingly widening gulf between
the cognitive scientists, speech pathologists, and educational psychologists on
the one hand, who make it their business to understand all aspects of human
learning, including the acquisition of reading, and education academics on the
other, whose self-selected remit in recent years, has largely been to promote
in pre-service teachers, a simplistic (at best) view of the process of learning
to read. This sits alongside a misplaced belief that so-called authentic children’s literature and
immersion in text and spoken language, a smattering of sight (irregular/high-frequency)
words, with some incidental, light-touch phonics sprinkled on top will
suffice to transform all young children into proficient readers. This belief is
patently incorrect.
Teachers should be the most expert professionals in schools about the teaching of reading, the early identification of children who are falling behind, and optimal ways to support such students to steer them back on track. Evidence, however, indicates that this is not the case, because their core knowledge of how language works is under-done, and teachers do not feel well-prepared by their initial teacher education (ITE) for these tasks (Meeks et al., 2018). Education academics have wilfully ignored the body of scientific knowledge (derived mainly from cognitive psychology research) about how children learn to read, and in so-doing, have robbed their graduates of their rightful status as well-informed, evidence-based practitioners. True professionals uphold high ethical standards by having the tools to question assumptions and maintain up-to-date practice in line with the best available evidence about what works in the majority of cases. Instead of commitment to scientific rigour and accountability, however, we see a “choose your own adventure” approach to early reading instruction, such that it is possible to visit two adjacent Foundation (“Reception” in some states) year classrooms in the one school, and observe vastly different approaches to reading instruction, both technically aligned to the accommodatingly elastic curriculum. Imagine the corollary in a hospital, where staff in two adjacent wards did their own thing with respect to hand-washing, or in the airline industry where pilots where given free-rein to try out a few ideas of their own when landing Boeing 747s.
Teachers should be the most expert professionals in schools about the teaching of reading, the early identification of children who are falling behind, and optimal ways to support such students to steer them back on track. Evidence, however, indicates that this is not the case, because their core knowledge of how language works is under-done, and teachers do not feel well-prepared by their initial teacher education (ITE) for these tasks (Meeks et al., 2018). Education academics have wilfully ignored the body of scientific knowledge (derived mainly from cognitive psychology research) about how children learn to read, and in so-doing, have robbed their graduates of their rightful status as well-informed, evidence-based practitioners. True professionals uphold high ethical standards by having the tools to question assumptions and maintain up-to-date practice in line with the best available evidence about what works in the majority of cases. Instead of commitment to scientific rigour and accountability, however, we see a “choose your own adventure” approach to early reading instruction, such that it is possible to visit two adjacent Foundation (“Reception” in some states) year classrooms in the one school, and observe vastly different approaches to reading instruction, both technically aligned to the accommodatingly elastic curriculum. Imagine the corollary in a hospital, where staff in two adjacent wards did their own thing with respect to hand-washing, or in the airline industry where pilots where given free-rein to try out a few ideas of their own when landing Boeing 747s.
There is a science to effective reading instruction, in the same
way that there is a science to infection control, or to airline safety. All
interface with human judgement and the vagaries of human behaviour, but these take
their place behind scientific rigour and logic. Education, however, has been
allowed to thumb its nose at science and go its own way, engaging with jingoistic
time and resource-wasting fads in the process (learning styles, Brain Gym,
multiple intelligences, coloured overlays, growth mindset, brain-based
learning, to name a few), instead of bearing down and doing the necessary, though
sometimes difficult job of understanding and applying cognitive and linguistic science
evidence as this pertains to early reading instruction. Children and their
parents engage with school on the implicit assumption that the best available
evidence is going to underpin everyday instruction. Instead, parents
unwittingly buy a ticket in a lottery when their children start school. That
the rest of us look the other way when this basic contract of trust is
violated, is no longer excusable.
It is not acceptable to engage in parent-blame
regarding early oral language exposure, to “explain” the poor reading
achievement of some children. That is, however, the tactic employed by speakers
for the negative in the recent Phonics Debate. This begs the question, then, as
to exactly what the role of early years teachers is. According to the negative
team’s argument, this role is to witness (and perhaps take credit for) the
benefits of socio-economic status and the work done by parents in the
pre-school years. This not only displays a fundamental lack of understanding of
the nuanced relationship between oral language and early literacy, but it also
displaces responsibility for poor reading outcomes to children and their
parents. It is not the job of parents to teach children how to read. That is
the job of teachers. I wonder how teachers who are themselves parents of
struggling readers interpret this message?
Ironically, if there is any message at all
in the fact that some children start from behind with respect to their oral
language skills, it is not that their
poor reading outcomes can be dismissed and explained away on the basis of the
sub-standard genetic and/or environmental endowment parents have bestowed on
their children. Rather, it is that teachers need to engage in instructional
practices that accelerate the
progress of such children relative to their more advantaged peers. However, teachers who are ill-equipped to
understand the science of learning, working memory, linguistics, orthographic
mapping, and explicit teaching, have insufficient tools in their teaching
toolkits to meet the needs of the full-range of learners in their classrooms.
This must be experienced as a demoralising, self-perpetuating cycle for early
years teachers who approach reading instruction in line with the beliefs of the
negative team.
Oral language skills (e.g., phonological
and phonemic awareness, vocabulary, syntactic complexity, conversational and
narrative language skills, in receptive and expressive domains) are undoubtedly
the essential underpinning to the transition to literacy in the early years. However,
while oral language may be natural in an evolutionary
sense, it is no “set and forget” function in a developmental sense. It has long been known that children’s early
oral language exposure sits on a social gradient, such that children of higher
socio-economic status (SES) parents are typically significantly advantaged over
their lower-SES peers with respect to the nature and amount of language spoken
to them in the pre-school years (Hart & Risley, 1995; Hoff, 2003; Locke et al.,
2002; Roy & Chiat, 2013; Spencer et al., 2012; Weisleder & Fernald,
2013). Put simply, oral language skills are the
engine, and effective instruction is the fuel in the tank. The two are
inter-dependent and the quality of one interacts with, and influences the
quality of the other. The engine can be made more powerful, but not without the
fuel of high quality instruction, as evidenced by the fact that once children
become readers, their own reading is a significant source of new vocabulary
(Nippold, 2007).
In and of themselves, however, oral
language skills will not see children across the bridge from talking and listening
in the pre-school years, to reading, writing, and spelling in the early years
of school. As reading, writing, and spelling are biologically unnatural skills (Gough &
Hillinger, 1980), children
require specific instruction it order to master their intricacies and
inconsistencies. As noted recently by Treiman (2018) the uncritical
perpetuation by education academics, of the idea that reading to children turns
them into good readers has blindsided many in education to the merits of
explicit teaching. Explicit teaching, in turn, seems to be held back in reserve
for students requiring Tier 2 (remedial) support, rather than being a
front-line Tier 1 (universal) strategy designed to promote success for all
children. This is a folly.
Literacy builds on oral language, but differs from it in a
number of key ways. Oral language occurs in real time and typically (though not
invariably) in the context of interactions with others. It contains pauses,
hesitations, and false starts, and is generally less complex syntactically than
written language, where the reader is able to run their eyes back over sections
of text as many times as are needed in order to confirm understanding. In
written language, punctuation is used to augment meaning and clues about emotion.
A question mark implies a rising intonation, a full-stop signifies falling
intonation, and an exclamation mark alerts the reader to surprise or alarm. In
oral language, all of these phenomena are conveyed by speakers through intonation
and prosodic contour. Written text is not simply oral language written down,
and so familiarity with the spoken modality will only go so far in assisting
children to succeed in a skill set that does not come naturally.
In spite of some education academics’
protestations to the contrary, 1970s Whole Language thinking is not buried deep
in the ITE archive in Australia. It is alive and well and sees the light of day,
every day in classrooms around Australia (and elsewhere). While it may have a re-badged name,
such as Balanced Literacy, scratch
the surface, and you will find the ancestral instructional practices that were
promulgated fifty years ago by the likes of Goodman (1967) and more recently by
Smith (2004), through their promotion of the proposition that reading is a psycholinguistic guessing game.
Three-cueing (also called multi-cueing, or “Searchlights”), levelled,
predictable readers, and a blatant disregard for the instructional role of
decodable texts for beginning readers are all hallmarks of this in-perpetuity
legacy.
Rather than an education knowledge-gap
being the biggest hindrance to progress in early reading instruction, we must
grapple with a wasteful knowledge-translation
crisis. Such waste of knowledge is unforgivable and would not be tolerated
in other fields, where reliably-established changes in knowledge transform into
changes in practice as a matter of course. There is abundant evidence to show that
teachers (and in many cases their educators) in western nations such as
Australia, the US, Canada, and the UK typically have limited and superficial
knowledge of the linguistic basis of learning to read, and of the specific
linguistic constructs that underpin this (Binks-Cantrell et al., 2012; Fielding-Barnsley, 2010; Fielding-Barnsley & Purdie, 2005; Hammond,
2015; Joshi et al., 2009; Louden & Rohl, 2006; Mahar & Richdale, 2008;
Moats, 2009; Piasta et al., 2009; Podhajski , 2009; Reid Lyon & Weiser,
2009; Stark et al., 2015; Tetley & Jones, 2014; Washburn et al., 2011; Washburn &
Mulcahy, 2014). Even more worryingly, there is evidence of an inverse
relationship between teacher language knowledge and self-confidence with
respect to this knowledge (Stark et al., 2015). This severely undermines the
extent to which the community can be confident in teachers as experts and
professionals. The persisting strong-hold of Whole Language-based ideologies
and practices in ITE and classrooms, means that early years teachers are stuck
in a 1970s time-warp, while practitioners in fields such as speech pathology
and educational and developmental psychology have moved on, using twenty-first
century knowledge in their everyday work. Many teachers eventually have their
own epiphany about gaps in their understanding and practices, entering into
long, expensive journeys of discovery to claim their rightful body of
knowledge. It should not need to be so.
Is it time that we faced the sobering
reality that in the main in Australia, neither classroom teachers nor education
academics are sufficiently knowledgeable about the cognitive science
underpinning effective reading instruction? That neither classroom teachers nor
education academics have an in-depth understanding of how language works “under
the bonnet”, in the same way that a mechanic needs to understand the inner
workings of a car’s engine in order to be able to tune, maintain, and repair it?
In so doing, do we also need to accept that the conflation of oral language
with the acquisition of its biologically unnatural cousin, reading, betrays a
serious lack of knowledge about the cognitive processes underpinning both oral
language and early reading?
Falling ATARs for entry into teaching
courses in recent years mean that pre-service and recently graduated teachers
are less likely than ever before to have an explicit grasp of how language
works, and they will find it harder to learn this information while at
university (particularly if it is only alluded to in the most general, if not dismissive
manner). Through a steady but insidious process of mutual attraction and
dependence, attenuated ITE curricula and less-prepared students have been drawn
to each other, ever more compellingly. Like the proverbial boiling frog, this
has not been obvious to those within, but is painfully evident to observers.
Unfortunately, but inevitably, calls for
effective reading instruction are political
– in the sense that under-done reading skills are one of the surest paths to
social marginalisation and economic disadvantage across the lifespan. Youth
justice centres, adult prisons, public housing waiting lists, and mental health
and substance abuse services all include an over-representation of citizens who
did not learn to read in the early years of school (Snow, 2016). This is a
social justice issue and if that makes it political, then so be it. Decades of
presenting evidence and advocating for its translation into ITE and classroom
practice have not resulted in change in education faculties or classrooms.
Hence, like climate change and marriage equality, equitable access to
evidence-based reading instruction needs to be debated and resolved in the
political and public arena.
Flat earth thinking about reading
instruction is not excusable in 2018. Sustained failure to adopt scientific
knowledge and transmit it to its rightful custodians and beneficiaries is simply
cosying up with pseudoscience.
It’s time to move from the Reading Dark Ages to
a Reading Renaissance.
References
Binks-Cantrell, E., Washburn, E.
K., Joshi, R. M., & Hougen, M. (2012). Peter Effect in the preparation of
reading teachers. Scientific Studies of
Reading, 16(6), 526-536.
Fielding-Barnsley, R. (2010).
Australian pre-service teachers' knowledge of phonemic awareness and phonics in
the process of learning to read. Australian
Journal of Learning Difficulties, 15(1), 99-110.
Fielding-Barnsley, R., &
Purdie, N. (2005). Teachers' attitude to and knowledge of metalinguistics in
the process of learning to read. Asia-Pacific
Journal of Teacher Education, 33(1), 65-76.
Goodman,
K.S. (1967) Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game, Journal of the Reading Specialist, 6(4), 126-135.
Gough, P., & Hillinger, M. (1980). Learning to
read: an unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, 1–17.
Hammond, L. (2015). Early childhood educators’
perceived and actual metalinguistic knowledge, beliefs and enacted practice
about teaching early reading. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 58(3),
853–864.
Hart, B. & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful
Differences in Everyday Parenting and Intellectual Development in Young
American Children. Baltimore: Brookes.
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence:
Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal
speech. Child Development (74), 1368-1378.
Joshi, R. M., Binks, E., Hougen, M., Dahlgren, M. E., Ocker-Dean, E.,
& Smith, D. L. (2009). Why elementary teachers might be inadequately
prepared to teach reading. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 42(5), 392-402.
Locke, A., Ginsborg, J. & Peers, I. (2002).
Development and disadvantage: Implications for the early years and
beyond. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
37, 3-15.
Louden, W., & Rohl, M. (2006).
“Too many theories and not enough instruction”: Perceptions of preservice
teacher preparation for literacy teaching in Australian schools. Literacy, 40(2), 66-78.
Mahar, N. E., & Richdale, A. L. (2008). Primary teachers' linguistic
knowledge and perceptions of early literacy instruction. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 13(1), 17-37.
Meeks, L.,
Stephenson, J., Kemp, C., & Madelaine, A. (2018). How well-prepared are
pre-service teachers to teach early reading? A systematic review of the
literature. Australian Journal of
Learning Difficulties, 21(2), 69-98.
Moats, L. (2009). Still wanted: Teachers with knowledge of language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(5),
387 – 391.
Nippold, M.A. (2007). Later Language
Development. School-Age Children, Adolescents, and Young Adults (3rd ed.). Austin, Tx: Pro-Ed.
Piasta, S. B., Connor, C. M.,
Fishman, B. J., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Teachers' knowledge of literacy
concepts, classroom practices, and student reading growth. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(3), 224-248.
Podhajski, B., Mather, N., Nathan, J., & Sammons, J. (2009).
Professional development in scientifically based reading instruction teacher
knowledge and reading outcomes. Journal
of Learning Disabilities, 42(5), 403-417.
Reid Lyon, G., & Weiser, B. (2009). Teacher knowledge, instructional
expertise, and the development of reading proficiency. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(5), 475-480.
Roy, P. & Chiat, S. (2013). Teasing apart disadvantage from disorder. The case of poor language. In C.R. Marshall (ed.) Current Issues in Developmental Disorders (pp. 125–150). London: Psychology Press.
Roy, P. & Chiat, S. (2013). Teasing apart disadvantage from disorder. The case of poor language. In C.R. Marshall (ed.) Current Issues in Developmental Disorders (pp. 125–150). London: Psychology Press.
Smith,
F. (2004). Understanding Reading: A Psycholinguistic Analysis of Reading and
Learning to Read. Routledge.
Snow,
P.C. (2016). Elizabeth Usher Memorial Lecture: Language is literacy is
language. Positioning Speech Language Pathology in education policy, practice,
paradigms, and polemics. International
Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 18(3), 216-228.
Spencer, S., Clegg, J., &
Stackhouse, J. (2012). Language and social disadvantage: A comparison of the
language abilities of adolescents from two different socio-economic
areas. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
47, 3, 274–284.
Stark, H. L., Snow, P. C., Eadie, P. A., &
Goldfeld, S. R. (2015). Language and reading instruction in early years’
classrooms: The knowledge and self-rated ability of Australian teachers. Annals
of Dyslexia, 1-27.
Tetley, D., & Jones, C.
(2014). Pre-service teachers’ knowledge of language concepts: Relationships to
field experiences. Australian Journal of
Learning Difficulties, 19(1), 17-32.
Treiman, R. (2018). What
research tells us about reading instruction. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(1), 1-4.
Washburn, E. K., Joshi, R. M., & Cantrell, E. B. (2011). Are
preservice teachers prepared to teach struggling readers? Annals of Dyslexia, 61(1), 21-43.
Washburn, E. K., & Mulcahy, C.
A. (2014). Expanding preservice teachers' knowledge of the English language:
Recommendations for teacher educators. Reading
& Writing Quarterly, 30(4), 328-347.
Weisleder, A. & Fernald, A.
(2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens
processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11),
2143-2152.
Pam, I just wanted to thank you for crystallising the key issues here. This is one of the clearest evaluations of the core messages I have seen.
ReplyDeleteSuperb summation!
ReplyDeleteDear Pam, you write: “his sits alongside a misplaced belief that so-called authenticchildren’s literature and immersion in text and spoken language, a smattering of sight (irregular/high-frequency) words, with some incidental, light-touch phonics sprinkled on top will suffice to transform all young children into proficient readers. This belief is patently incorrect.”.
ReplyDeleteI thought I would point out to your followers that the current empirical evidence from 12 meta-analyses does not support this claim. Rather, the evidence suggests that the systematic phonics is equally effective as non-systematic phonics as practiced in whole language for example. Note, the evidence does not support whole language either, it suggests that we need to look for something else. Below is a link to a short summary of the evidence that contains a link to a detailed review.
There really is no evidence that systematic phonics works better than current alternative methods. I wish researchers who repeatedly advocate systematic phonics would engage with the evidence I summarize. See: https://jeffbowers.blogs.bristol.ac.uk/blog/phonics/
Jeff
Hello (again) Jeff. It's not surprising to me that systematic phonics, as practiced in the past 20 years (since the US National Reading Panel called for it) is doing no better than the Whole Language method that preceded it. The reason I'm not surprised is that Balanced Literacy folks have substituted analytic phonics for systematic phonics.
DeleteAnalytic phonics can be systematic but it takes about 6 years to cover the entire code “analytically.” That’s because the analysis depends on children first gathering a large cache of consciously-memorized sight words in order to have something to analyze. With their sight words in hand, they set out to “discover” the underlying code.
That’s no way to teach reading. Most kids give up on reading precisely in the initial two years of instruction, which consists in nothing but a blur of sight word memorization, invented spellings, guessing based on pictures, and “reading” from repetitive “little books” where only a single word changes as the child turns the page. NONE of this is logical and many kids give up during this period. Hence, no surprise that systematic phonics is no better than Whole Language.
You suggest “we need to look for something else.” Agreed. That “something else” is synthetic phonics – an approach that starts logically rather with sight words and guessing. It starts with individual phonemes and graphemes and then synthesizes those sounds and letters into whole words.
You (and other readers of this blog) can learn more about it by downloading my FREE phonics books at parkerphonics.com and/or by following me on Twitter (@parkerphonics).
Stephen Parker
I mentioned Stephen Parker and his book in a recent article about all the activity in phonics (Never quiet on the phonics front). I can vouch that his emphasis on early learning is repeated by many others. For years, the Education Establishment said children couldn't learn to read until seven or eight or nine and didn't need to. The war on reading took many forms and this was one of them. Apparently the age of three of four is fine. This highlights the disgraceful scam of our so-called literacy experts as they casually let children stay illiterate.
DeleteDear Stephen, across 12 meta-analyses there is little or no evidence that systematic phonics is better than non-systematic phonics. Your comment regarding “balanced literacy” is irrelevant here – none of these meta-analyses confused systematic phonics with balanced literacy. If readers are interested in the evidence, I suggest that they read my review and the associated blogpost. Again see: https://jeffbowers.blogs.bristol.ac.uk/blog/phonics/
ReplyDeleteYou can also find papers published in peer reviewed journals detailing alternatives to systematic phonics here: https://jeffbowers.blogs.bristol.ac.uk/publications-edu-literacy/
New development. New York Times supports phonics. I thought the word "Renaissance" was too optimistic. But this is certainly a positive sign in support of optimism.
ReplyDeleteMy commentary is called "K-12: the Politics of Illiteracy." After all, we have to explain the decades of silence when the Times pretended to be agnostic. Surely they knew everything we know now.
http://www.renewamerica.com/columns/bprice/181101
------------------------------------------------------
Link to Times article: https://www.nytimes.com/2018/10/26/opinion/sunday/phonics-teaching-reading-wrong-way.html
----------------------------
--------------------------------------------------